Cue-dependent forgetting
Cue-dependent forgetting, or retrieval failure, is the failure to recall information without memory cues.[1] The term either pertains to semantic cues, state-dependent cues or context-dependent cues.
Upon performing a search for files in a computer, its memory is scanned for words. Relevant files containing this word or string of words does then show up. This is not how memory in the human mind works. Instead, information stored in the memory is retrieved by way of association with other memories. Some memories can not be recalled by simply thinking about them. Rather, one must think about something associated with it.
For example, if someone tries and fails to recollect the memories he had about a vacation he went on, and someone mentions the fact that he hired a classic car during this vacation, this may make him remember all sorts of things from that trip, such as what he ate there, where he went and what books he read.
Cue typologies
Semantic cues
An experiment from 1966 showed that people remember a group of words better if they are within the same theme category. Such words that generate recall by association are known as semantic cues.[2] If the sound of the word is emphasized during the encoding process, a cue that could be used could also put emphasis on the phonetic quality of the word.[3]
State-dependent cues
State-dependent cues are governed by the state of mind and being at the time of encoding. The emotional or mental state of the person, such as being inebriated, drugged, upset, anxious or happy are key cues.[4]
Context-dependent cues
Research suggests there is also context-dependent cues which is dependent on environment and situation.[5] In an experiment conducted in 1975 deep sea divers were divided into two groups. Each group had the same list of 36 unrelated words to learn. But one of the groups learned it 15 feet underwater, and the other on land. Both groups then had recall those words underwater as well as on land. The divers who had learnt the words on land recalled 38% when tested on land and 21% when under water. Those who had learnt the words under water recalled 21% on the beach and 32% when under water.
Memory retrieval can be facilitated or triggered by replication of the context in which the memory was encoded. Such conditions include weather, company, location, smell of a particular odor, hearing a certain song, even taste can sometimes act as a cue. For example, students sometimes fail to recall diligently studied material when an examination room's environmental conditions differ significantly from the room or place where learning occurred. Students may consider studying under conditions that resemble an examination center which could boost their recall during actual exams.
Psychologists that have researched context dependent recall include Abernethy (1940).[6]
See also
References
- ↑ Pastorino, Ellen E.; Doyle-Portillo, Susann M. (2011). What is Psychology? Essentials (2nd ed.). Cengage Learning. p. 228. ISBN 1111834156.
- ↑ Tulving, Endel; Pearlstone, Zena (1966). "Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 5 (4): 381–391. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80048-8.
- ↑ Weiten, Wayne (2012). Psychology: Themes and Variations (9th ed.). Cengage Learning. p. 277. ISBN 9781111354749.
- ↑ Goodwin, D.; Powell, B.; Bremer, D.; Hoine, H.; Stern, J. (1969). "Alcohol and recall: state-dependent effects in man". Science. 163 (3873): 1358–1360. doi:10.1126/science.163.3873.1358. PMID 5774177.
- ↑ Godden, D; Baddeley, A. (1975). "Context-Dependent Memory in Two Natural Environments: On Land and Underwater". British Journal of Psychology. 66 (3): 325–331. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x.
- ↑ Abernethy, Ethel Mary (1940). "The Effect of Changed Environmental Conditions Upon the Results of College Examinations". The Journal of Psychology. 10 (2): 293–301. doi:10.1080/00223980.1940.9917005.
Further reading
- Tulving, Endel (1974). "Cue-Dependent Forgetting: When we forget something we once knew, it does not necessarily mean that the memory trace has been lost; it may only be inaccessible". American Scientist. 62 (1): 74–82. JSTOR 27844717.