Daniel D. Langleben
Daniel D. Langleben is a psychiatrist, professor, and scientific researcher at the University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia. He pioneered a technique for using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) as a means of measuring lie detection.[1] He is currently a Professor of Psychiatry at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine. He has also studied the brain effects of packaging and advertising[2] and how infants' cuteness motivates caretaking in adults.[3]
Biography
He graduated from the Hebrew University of Jerusalem in 1982, and from the Hebrew University Hadassah Medical School in 1989. He completed his internship at the Medical College of Pennsylvania and then residencies in Psychiatry and Radiology at the Mount Sinai Hospital.[4]
fMRI and lie detection
2001 study
Langleben was inspired to test lie detection while he was at Stanford University studying the effects of a drug on children with Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD).[5] He found that these children have a more difficult time inhibiting the truth.[6] He postulated that lying requires increased brain activity compared to truth because the truth must be suppressed, essentially creating more work for the brain. In 2001, he published his first work with lie detection using a modified form of the Guilty Knowledge Test, which is sometimes used in polygraph tests.[7] The subjects, right-handed, male college students, were given a card and a Yes/No handheld clicker.[8] They were told to lie to a computer asking questions while they underwent a brain scan only when the question would reveal their card.[9][10] The subjects were given $20 for participating, and told they would receive more money if they deceived the computer; however, none did.[11]
His studies showed that the inferior and superior prefrontal and anterior cingulate gyri and the parietal cortex, showed increased activity during deception.[12] In 2002, he licensed his methods for lie detection to the No Lie MRI company located in San Diego, California.
Critiques
Critiques of this technique point out that fMRI does not actually measure lying, only the increased brain activity that occurs when one is lying. Using fMRI for lie detection could then lead to false positives produced by anxiety or other causes.[13]
Another concern is that a "lie" is not always clear-cut, and may be a complex concept. More complex types of deception may not be detected by imaging techniques.[14]
See also
References
- ↑ Dennis Coon; John Mitterer (1 January 2013). Psychology: A Journey. Cengage Learning. p. 72. ISBN 1-133-95782-X.
- ↑ Wang, AL; Ruparel, K; Loughead, JW; Strasser, AA; Blady, SJ; Lynch, KG; Romer, D; Cappella, JN; Lerman, C; Langleben, DD (2013). "Content matters: neuroimaging investigation of brain and behavioral impact of televised anti-tobacco public service announcements". J Neurosci. 33 (17): 7420–7427. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3840-12.2013. PMC 3773220. PMID 23616548.
- ↑ Glocker, ML; Langleben, DD; Ruparel, K; Loughead, JW; Valdez, JN; Griffin, MD; Sachser, N; Gur, RC (Jun 2009). "Baby schema modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous women". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 106 (22): 9115–9119. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811620106. PMC 2690007. PMID 19451625.
- ↑ "Daniel D. Langleben". University of Pennsylvania. Retrieved 9 July 2014.
- ↑ Silberman, Steve (2006). "Don't Even Think About Lying". Wired. pp. Issue 14.01. Retrieved 9 July 2014.
- ↑ Prospect: Politics, Essay, Review. C. Seaford. October 2009.
- ↑ Zack Lynch; Byron Laursen (21 July 2009). The Neuro Revolution: How Brain Science Is Changing Our World. St. Martin's Press. p. 29. ISBN 978-1-4299-8823-0.
- ↑ Bonnier Corporation (August 2002). Popular Science. Bonnier Corporation. p. 58. ISSN 0161-7370.
- ↑ Boy Scouts of America, Inc. (January 2005). Boys' Life. Boy Scouts of America, Inc. p. 11. ISSN 0006-8608.
- ↑ Committee on Science, Technology, Law; Policy and Global Affairs; Committee on the Development of the Third Edition of the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence; Federal Judicial Center; National Research Council (26 September 2011). Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence:: Third Edition. National Academies Press. p. 803. ISBN 978-0-309-21421-6.
- ↑ Allan Pease; Barbara Pease (1 January 2004). Why Men Don't Have a Clue and Women Always Need More Shoes: The Ultimate Guide to the Opposite Sex. Broadway Books. p. 272. ISBN 978-0-7679-1610-3.
- ↑ Langleben, Daniel (2002). "Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study". NeuroImage. 15 (3): 727–32. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1003. PMID 11848716.
- ↑ Spence, Sean A. (February 2008). "Playing Devil's Advocate: The case against fMRI lie detection". Legal and Criminological Psychology. 12 (1): 11–25. doi:10.1348/135532507X251597.
- ↑ National Academy of Engineering (2 September 2004). Emerging Technologies and Ethical Issues in Engineering:: Papers from a Workshop, October 14-15, 2003. National Academies Press. p. 61. ISBN 978-0-309-16572-3.
Selected Publications
- Langleben, Daniel D. (1 February 2008). "Detection of deception with fMRI: Are we there yet?". Legal and Criminological Psychology. 13 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1348/135532507X251641.
- Moriarty, Jane C.; Langleben, Daniel D. (May 1, 2013). "Using Brain Imaging for Lie Detection: Where Science, Law and Research Policy Collide". Psychol Public Policy Law. 19 (2): 222–234. doi:10.1037/a0028841. PMC 3680134. PMID 23772173.
- Wang, An-Li; et al. (April 2013). "Content matters: neuroimaging investigation of brain and behavioral impact of televised anti-tobacco public service announcements.". Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (17): 7420–7427. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3840-12.2013. PMC 3773220. PMID 23616548.
- Langleben DD; Hakun JG; Seelig D; Wang AL; Ruparel K & Bilker WB (2016). "Polygraphy and functional magnetic resonance imaging in lie detection: a controlled blind comparison using the concealed information test.". J Clin Psychiatry. doi:10.4088/JCP.15m09785. PMID 26794034.
External links
- UPenn profile, contains various links to relevant media and publications