Daniel Wegner

Daniel Wegner
Born June 28, 1948
Calgary, Alberta, Canada
Died July 5, 2013(2013-07-05) (aged 65)
Winchester, Massachusetts, United States
Residence United States
Fields Social Psychology
Institutions Harvard University, Trinity University, University of Virginia
Alma mater Michigan State University
Known for ironic process theory, transactive memory, experimental study of free will
Website
www.danielwegner.com

Daniel Merton Wegner (June 28, 1948 – July 5, 2013) was an American social psychologist. He was a professor of psychology at Harvard University and a fellow of both the American Association for the Advancement of Science and the American Academy of Arts and Sciences. He was known for applying experimental psychology to the topics of mental control (for example ironic process theory) and conscious will,[1][2] and for originating the study of transactive memory and action identification. In The Illusion of Conscious Will and other works, he argued controversially that the human sense of free will is an illusion.

Career

Wegner was born in Calgary, Alberta, Canada.[1] He enrolled in a physics degree at Michigan State University but changed to psychology, going on to an M.A. and then a PhD. After gaining his doctorate in 1974, he spent sixteen years teaching at Trinity University, becoming a full Professor in 1985. From 1990 to 2000, he researched and taught at the University of Virginia, after which he joined the faculty at Harvard University.[3]

Awards

In 2011, Wegner was awarded the William James Fellow Award by the Association for Psychological Science, the Distinguished Scientific Contribution Award by the American Psychological Association, and the Distinguished Scientist Award by the Society of Experimental Social Psychology. In 2012, he was awarded the Donald T. Campbell Award by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology (SPSP). Furthermore, shortly after Wegner's death in 2013, SPSP announced that its annually awarded Theoretical Innovation Prize would henceforth be known as the Daniel M. Wegner Theoretical Innovation Prize to honor Wegner's memory and his innovative work.

Research

Ironic process theory

Wegner and colleagues performed a series of experiments in which people tried to suppress thoughts, for example by attempting not to think of a white bear. That work revealed that attempting not to think of a topic often backfires, resulting in high rates of intrusive thoughts about the topic. Wegner coined the term "ironic mental processes" for this effect, which is also known more commonly as the "white bear phenomenon". The effect contributes to various psychological challenges and disorders. Smokers who try not to think about cigarettes find it harder to give up. People who suppress thoughts that may cause an anxiety reaction often make those thoughts more intrusive.[4][5] Wegner found that the ironic effect is stronger when people are stressed or depressed.[6]

The illusion of conscious will

Wegner conducted a series of experiments in which people experience an illusion of control, feeling that their will shapes events which are actually determined by someone else.[1] He argued controversially that the ease with which this illusion can be created shows that the everyday feeling of conscious will is an illusion or a "construction"[7] and that this illusion of mental causation is "the mind's best trick".[8] Wegner defined conscious will as a function of priority (the thought must come before the action), consistency (the thought must be consistent with the action), and exclusivity (the thought cannot be accompanied with other causes). [9] He argued that, although people may feel that conscious intentions drive much of their behavior, in reality both behavior and intentions are the product of other, unconscious mental processes.[10] Wegner's research agreed with previous findings by Benjamin Libet regarding brain readiness potential and concluded that his own findings were "compatible with the idea that brain events cause intention and action, whereas conscious intention itself may not cause action." [11] The notion that we believe that our actions are caused by our conscious volition was observed to be false by Wegner's findings. There is an unconscious element in the mind that plays a pivotal role in an individual's seemingly conscious volition and ultimate action.

Apparent mental causation

Wegner argued that the feeling of intention is something attributed "after the fact" according to three principles: consistency, exclusivity, and priority.[12] The principle of consistency states that if the content of one's thoughts is relevant to one's action, then a feeling of control will occur. The exclusivity principle holds that one must not believe there to be an outside influence or cause to feel as though an action was intended. Finally, the priority principle requires the thought to occur right before the action to produce the illusion of free will.

He did not claim that conscious thought cannot in principle cause action, merely that any connection between conscious thought and action should be determined by scientific enquiry, and not by unreliable introspection and feelings.[8]

Transactive memory

Main article: Transactive memory

The concept of transactive memory was proposed by Wegner in 1985. A transactive memory system is a system through which groups collectively encode, store, and retrieve knowledge.[13] Transactive memory suggests an analysis not only of how couples and families in close relationships coordinate memory and tasks at home, but how teams, larger groups and organizations come to develop a "group mind",[13] a memory system that is more complex and potentially more effective than that of any of the individuals that comprise it.

According to Wegner, a transactive memory system consists of the knowledge stored in each individual's memory combined with metamemory containing information regarding the different teammate's domains of expertise.[14] Just as the individual's metamemory allows him to be aware of what information is available for retrieval, so does the transactive memory system provide teammates with information regarding the knowledge they have access to within the team.[15] Group members learn who knowledge experts are and how to access expertise through communicative processes. In this way, a transactive memory system can provide the group members with more and better knowledge than any individual could access on his or her own.

Death

Trinity University announced Wegner's death on Friday, July 5, 2013, at his home in Massachusetts, of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis.[16] He was survived by his wife and two daughters. [17]

Books

Notes and references

  1. 1 2 3 Blackmore, Susan J. (15 November 2005). "Daniel Wegner". Conversations on consciousness. Oxford University Press. pp. 245–257. ISBN 978-0-19-280622-2. Retrieved 21 March 2011.
  2. "More than good intentions: Holding fast to faith in free will", The New York Times 31 December 2002.
  3. Profile of Dan Wegner, Institute of Psychology, University of Freiburg
  4. Baumeister, Roy F.; Bushman, Brad J. (16 October 2009). Social Psychology and Human Nature. Cengage Learning. p. 135. ISBN 978-0-495-60133-3. Retrieved 21 March 2011.
  5. Schacter, Daniel L. (1996). Searching for memory: the brain, the mind, and the past. Basic Books. p. 340. ISBN 978-0-465-07552-2. Retrieved 21 March 2011.
  6. Pennebaker, James W. (1997). Opening up: the healing power of expressing emotions. Guilford Press. pp. 59–. ISBN 978-1-57230-238-9. Retrieved 21 March 2011.
  7. Nadelhoffer, Thomas (11 June 2010). Moral Psychology: Historical and Contemporary Readings. John Wiley and Sons. p. 236. ISBN 978-1-4051-9019-0. Retrieved 21 March 2011.
  8. 1 2 Wegner, Daniel M. (2003). "The mind's best trick: how we experience conscious will" (PDF). Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (2): 65–69. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00002-0. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-05-22.
  9. Wegner, D.M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480-492.
  10. O'Shaughnessy, John; O'Shaughnessy, Nicholas J. (10 December 2007). The Undermining of Beliefs in the Autonomy and Rationality of Consumers. Routledge. p. 3. ISBN 978-0-415-77323-2. Retrieved 21 March 2011.
  11. Wegner, D.M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480-492.
  12. Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480-492.
  13. 1 2 Wegner, D. M., Giuliano, T., & Hertel, P. (1985). Cognitive interdependence in close relationships. In W. J. Ickes (Ed.), Compatible and incompatible relationships (pp. 253–276). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  14. Wegner, D. M. (1995). A computer network model of human transactive memory. Social Cognition, 13, 319–339.
  15. Wegner, D. M. (1986). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In B.Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp. 185–205). New York: Springer-Verlag
  16. Marquard, Bryan (2013-07-12). "Daniel M. Wegner, 65; Harvard social psychologist unraveled mysteries of thought and memory". The Boston Globe. Retrieved 2016-01-04. Dr. Wegner ... died last Friday in his Winchester home of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. He was 65.
  17. "Trinity University | Daniel Wegner". Web.trinity.edu. Retrieved 2013-07-12.

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 8/6/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.