Height and intelligence

The study of height and intelligence is the analysis of how variations in height affect human intelligence. Some epidemiological research on the subject has shown that there is a small but statistically significant positive correlation between height and intelligence after controlling for socioeconomic class and parental education.[1] This correlation arises in both the developed and developing world and persists across age groups. There is no scientific consensus on why this correlation arises, but one possible explanation is that height may act as a biomarker for nutritional status and health during intellectual development. One such theory argues that since height strongly correlates with white and gray matter volume, it may act as a biomarker for cerebral development which itself mediates intelligence.[2] Competing explanations include that certain genetic factors may influence both height and intelligence,[3] or that both height and intelligence may be affected in similar ways by adverse environmental exposures during development. Other explanations further qualify the positive correlation between height and intelligence, suggesting that because the correlation becomes weaker with higher socioeconomic class and education level, environmental factors could partially override any genetic factors affecting both characteristics.[4]

The Origin of the question

First inquiries into the correlation of height and intelligence came within the study of development in schoolchildren. William Porter was the first person to conduct a study to find a relationship between the physiology of children and their intelligence.[5] The motive for this research was to attempt to predict the potential “dullness” or “precocity” of children based on simple measurements teachers could make.[6] He did in fact find a correlation between body size and the learning level of children, but did not focus specifically on height.

More recent studies have continued the research into a correlation between height and intelligence, but again were often not directly related to height and intelligence. Some of the earlier large studies cited for height and intelligence are the Scottish Mental Surveys in 1932 and 1947. However, the studies were largely meant to analyze the genetic and environmental contributions to cognitive ability differences. Height (and weight) were added to provide a multivariate analysis.[7]

In effort to better understand this association, numerous other studies were thus carried out. These studies either expanded upon the association or sought to find an explanation for it.

The study by Douglas et al in 1965 addresses the Scottish study and sought to test if the association reflected a linkage between development of the brain and/or emotional development and the development of the rest of the body.[8] Similarly, in 1986, Wilson et al wanted to study if there is a longitudinal relation between height and intelligence.[9]

Nonetheless, as these studies were unable to provide satisfying results and explanations for the correlation, interest in it persists in the 21st century.

In 2014 another study was carried out by a team of researchers at Edinburgh University, Scotland, motivated by the understanding that both height and intelligence test scores are predictors of better health outcomes and mortality. The study was constructed to better identify if there are any shared phenotypic and genetic influences from height and intelligence in determining health outcomes and mortality.[10]

Definition of intelligence

Main article: Human intelligence

Human intelligence can be measured according to an extensive number of tests and criteria, ranging from academic, social, and emotional fields. There is no clear definition for what "intelligence" is in humans. There are several theories that define different categories of intelligence and associate traits, instead of a single general ability. In most of the studies, intelligence quotient (IQ) tests were used to measure a subject's mental age, which was checked for possible correlation with height.[9][10] While the use of IQ tests are highly debated among scientists as an accurate measurement of intelligence, they provide a quantitative and normal distribution to compare cognitive abilities among people.

The most common IQ tests are the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) for individuals over the age of 16 and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) for individuals aged between 6 and 16 years old. Many of the studies performed on the relationship between physical stature and intelligence used one of these tests in order to gauge relative cognitive ability based on the age of the participants.[11] In addition to IQ tests, some of the studies that were performed on children use academic performance as a measure of intelligence through standardized tests such as the Wide Range Achievement Test.

Correlation: studies and methodologies

A 1986 study of 13,887 American youths aged 6 to 17 years old sought to examine the correlation between height and intelligence. Height was normalized for age and sex, and intelligence was measured with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children and the Wide Range Achievement Test, measuring intellectual development (IQ) and academic achievement, respectively. Subjects were re-examined 2 to 5 years later. Both the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children and the Wide Range Achievement Test were found to significantly correlate with height. However, change in height was found not to be correlated with IQ score.[12]

In 2000 another study found a similar correlation. 127 growth-restricted and 32 non-growth-restricted children aged 9 to 24 months were put on a two-year "randomized trial of nutritional supplementation and psychosocial stimulation." Eight years later, the subjects' growth, IQ, and cognitive functions were measured. IQ was also measured using the Weschler Intelligence Scales. As in the 1986 study, it was found that supplementation to growth was not correlated with cognitive ability. Only psychosocial stimulation was found to increase IQ scores of subjects, and growth-restricted children had lower IQ scores than non-growth-restricted children, lending more evidence to the long-term cognitive consequences of growth restriction.[13]

These studies confirmed results found by JM Tanner in 1966. This study found that among children of school age, there is a correlation of about 0.15-0.25 between height and score on the 11+ test, an examination administered to students in England. It was found that this correlation decreases with age, but does not completely disappear; in samples of young adults, correlations of up to 0.2 were found. This study also found that “the greater the number of children in the family the lower their height and the less their scores in mental tests”, and attributes the effect “to co-advancement [of height and cognitive ability] and disappears when maturity is reached.” The correlation between number of children and intelligence was found to be stronger in poor families than in rich ones, which contributed to the conclusion that there is a strong correlation between height and mental ability between children in different socio-economic groups that persists in adulthood.[5]

A study was done by the Newcastle Thousands Families in 1947 to see if there was a correlation between birth weights and IQ at ages 9 and 13. There showed no correlation between birth weights and their IQs. However, height at the age of 9 played a major role in the IQ of the children (standardized regression coefficient b = 2.6, 95% CI 1.6-3.6, P < 0.0001). At the ages of 13, social economics played yet another larger role (b = 3.4, 95% CI 2.3-4.4, P = 0.001).[14]

With data from ten consecutive years of quantitative measurements on height and intelligence separated by girls and boys and gathered from the Harvard Growth Study, this compilation of analyses shows the difference in correlation between height and intelligence in the classes of girls and boys. Boys demonstrate no clear correlation between height at a certain age group and intelligence later on in life but girls do. There appears to be a clear link between intelligence levels at 10 and 11 with a girl’s height at the ages of 8 and 9 with other variables of note, i.e., socioeconomic status, age of first menstruation and ethnicity factoring into the correlation as well.[15]

A 1999 study that was conducted on a sample on 32,887 Swedish men, aged 18, free of growth defects showed that, by and large, shorter men (with 2 standard deviations below the mean) demonstrated poorer physical and psychological performance in the context of military service, with increased risk of musculoskeletal diagnoses. Additionally, increased height showed a relationship with increased mean intellectual performance and, under conditions of stress, shorter men showcased demonstrably worse leadership capability and psychological function.[16]

A 2011 model which incorporated assortative mating patterns into a bivariate model was used to account for height-to-intelligence factors that related to these mating habits as well as pleiotropic genetic influences when establishing the correlation between height and intelligence. Additionally, this team of researchers was responsible for using a dataset aggregated for Swedish male twins to explain both the genetic and environmental influences of the relationship between height and intelligence and height and capability to manage wartime stress.[17]

In 1989 the heights of male and female corporate managers and non-managers were measured in two studies with about 200 subjects. Both studies found a statistically significant correlation between managerial position and height; taller people were more likely to hold managerial positions, regardless of sex.[18]

A 2005 study used data on 1,181 identical twins and 1,412 fraternal twins, collected from Norwegian army files and Norwegian twin registries, in order to factor the correlation between intelligence test scores and standing height into environmental and genetic components. It used a structural equation model comparing the correlation between genetic, shared environmental and non-shared environmental factors and found that 59% of the height-intelligence correlation was due to shared environmental factors. Genes were found to contribute 35% of the correlation, and non-shared environmental factors contributed 6%, both statistically significant amounts.[19]

A 1991 study conducted on 76,111 Danish men sought to test the height-intelligence positive correlation on either extremes of height. The study defined two groups: the short group, composed of individuals below the 2nd percentile for height and the tall group, composed of individuals above the 98th percentile for height in Denmark. It found that the short group’s intelligence test score and educational level means lay about two-thirds of a standard deviation below the overall means, but suggested there appeared to be local factors that may have contributed negatively and significantly to the scores. In contrast, the study found the tall group scored about one-half standard deviation above the overall means, but suggested that both groups scored below what would be expected from a linear trend.[20]

A study from 1985 to 2005 was conducted on Dutch twin pairs from the ages 5 to 18 to establish a correlation factor.There was shown a positive correlation in childhood and early adolescence. These factors were used to gauge the intelligence in adults. The contributing factors for this study were mainly just genetics and full-scaled IQ.[21]

Explanations of the correlation

An individual’s height is determined by a complex interplay between genes and environment. Statistical analysis of multiple research studies have produced a correlation between height and intelligence. Various genetic and environmental factors may influence height, and the reasons are sought for any correlation with measured intelligence.

It has been suggested that increases in average height, in response to improved nutrition, have been accompanied by an increase in brain size, and is one explanation for an overall rise in measured intelligence among all tested populations that has been called the Flynn effect.[22]

Heritability estimates for height can be generated using comparisons among close relatives, such as between parents and children or siblings. To measure the effect of genetic inheritance, height comparisons are correlated with total shared genetic markers. Common percent values range between 60-80%,[23] with high variation attributed to differences in a population’s genetic history and environment (such as climate, nutrition, and lifestyle).

The most notable contributing environmental influences associated with the correlation of height and IQ include “diet, disease, psychosocial stress and inadequate cognitive stimulation”[24] Populations that live under stable conditions tend to have higher values of genetic influence, while populations susceptible to events of poor nutrition and illness show greater determination from their environment.[25]

An auxological approach that emphasizes multiple influences from all aspects of life, with special attention paid to prenatal and very early child development, seem to offer the most robust and accurate portrayal of overall growth and development. From these perspectives, we can better understand if the correlation persists, especially within developing countries prone to environmental uncertainties.[26]

Men on average are taller and have a higher IQ than women, probably as a result of the height-intelligence correlation (if height is controlled for, women have a higher average IQ).[27] The Trivers–Willard hypothesis suggests that female mammals are able to adjust offspring sex ratio in response to their maternal condition; a suggested generalization is that bigger and taller human parents are likely to have more sons than the average.[27][28] As those traits are considered desirable in mates of those respective genders, they confer an evolutionary advantage to offspring.[28] Height and intelligence may be genetically correlated as a result of this, because men tend to choose beautiful women as mates, and women tend to choose men who are either tall, intelligent, or both as mates.[27] Over time this might lead to an increased average height and intelligence of the population that is primarily inherited by men.

Height and intelligence may be correlated, but there is not significant proof of it being caused by genetics. There are many genes associated with genetic transmission of intelligence, but current theories of intelligence point to inheritance of intelligence from parents to be due to both genetic and environmental factors, especially as there is no concrete proof of the relationship between certain genes and intelligence.[29]

The correlation between height and intelligence could also be confounded by how cultural and psychological factors affect the expression of intelligence. It is impossible to create a culture-free test for intelligence,[30] as different societies value and cultivate different skill sets and ways of interpreting the world. For example, some cultures put more emphasis on hard work than being clever, making it difficult to accurately test and compare the intelligence of different groups within a population.[30]

There is evidence that intellectual development is influenced by the expectations placed on an individual.[31] In an experiment involving elementary school children, researchers found that when teachers believe students are above average intellect, these students tend to do better in school and receive higher IQ scores than students who are perceived as average or below average intellect, regardless of the actual competence of the students. This self-fulfilling prophecy caused by expectations is known as the Pygmalion effect,[31] and it may play have an effect on taller student who are generally viewed as more mature or dominant.[32]

Sexual selection and cross-trait assortative mating might also contribute to the correlation between height and intelligence.[33] Because both height and intelligence positively affect an individual's overall fitness, individuals generally look for these traits in potential partners. In other words, smart men are more likely to mate with tall women, and smart women are more likely to mate with tall men, as both traits are affected by perceptions of cross-trait attractiveness.[33]

Statistics

Across studies, the correlation coefficient of height and intelligence was generally found to be around 0.2,[34][35][36][37] indicating a positive association between height and intelligence which is weak but still statistically significant. A smaller within-family height-intelligence correlation of 0.10 was also found, indicating that both assortative mating and pleiotropy may be contributing causes for the general correlation.[34]

Height and intelligence are both polygenic traits, and as a result it is difficult to isolate possible causes for the variation in each of these traits individually, let alone causes for correlations between them.[34] Results regarding the relative responsibility of genetic and environmental factors in causing the correlation are far from conclusive; while some studies have arrived at similar estimates of the relative responsibility of genetic vs. environmental factors,[34][35] others have arrived at completely different estimates.[36]

It's also important to note that several of the recent studies confirming the correlation employed the standard bivariate ACE model,[34][35][36] which is extremely sensitive to assumptions reflected in parameters. For instance, two of these studies arrived at similar estimates for the relative responsibility of common environmental and genetic factors in causing the correlation: 59% and 59% for the former, 31% and 35% for the latter.[34][35] However, only slight modifications to the coefficient of assortative mating made the difference between the inferred percentage of genetic responsibility being as low as around 30% (the value the authors reported) to as high as 90%.[34] Further, another study using the same model even concluded that genetics alone could explain the correlation, without any influence from common environmental factors. This could have been because the studied population was relatively economically egalitarian, meaning access to resources like education and nutrition played less of a role than they might in other populations, or because of differences in how the researches handled the model and its parameters.[36] In any case, the model used in these studies is a problematic one which should be interpreted with caution, and the reported percentages of genetic vs. environmental responsibility should not be interpreted as precise figures.

See also

References

  1. Pearce, Mark S., Ian J. Dreary, Allan H. Young, and Louise Parker. "Growth in Early Life and Childhood IQ at Age 11 Years: The Newcastle Thousand Families Study." International Journal of Epidemiology 34.3 (2005): 673-77. Oxford Journals. Oxford Journals, 3 Mar. 2005. Web. Retrieved 9 Nov. 2016.
  2. Taki, Yasuyuki, Hiroshi Hashizume, Yuko Sassa, Hikaru Takeuchi, Michiko Asano, Kohei Asano, Yuka Kotozaki, Rui Nouchi, Kai Wu, Hiroshi Fukuda, and Ryuta Kawashima. "Correlation Among Body Height, Intelligence, and Brain Gray Matter Volume in Healthy Children." NeuroImage 59.2 (2012): 1023-027. Science Direct. Elsevier B.V. Web. Retrieved 14 Nov. 2016.
  3. Silventoinen, K., D. Posthuma, T. Van Beijsterveldt, M. Bartels, and D. I. Boomsma. "Genetic Contributions to the Association between Height and Intelligence: Evidence from Dutch Twin Data from Childhood to Middle Age." Genes, Brain and Behavior 5.8 (2006): 585-95. Nov. 2006. Web. Retrieved 14 Nov. 2016.
  4. Teasdale, T. W., T. I. Sorensen, and D. R. Owen. "Fall in Association of Height with Intelligence and Educational Level." BMJ 298.6683 (1989): 1292-293. PubMed Central. NCBI. Web. Retrieved 14 Nov. 2016.
  5. 1 2 Tanner, J. M. (1966). Galtonian eugenics and the study of growth: the relation of body size, intelligence test score, and social circumstances in children and adults. The Eugenics Review, 58(3), 122–135.
  6. Porter. W. (1893). The Physical Basis of Precocity and Dullness. Transaction of the Academy of Science of St. Louis 6 (7). 161-181.
  7. BENYAMIN, B. et al. “LARGE, CONSISTENT ESTIMATES OF THE HERITABILITY OF COGNITIVE ABILITY IN TWO ENTIRE POPULATIONS OF 11-YEAR OLD TWINS FROM SCOTTISH MENTAL SURVEYS OF 1932 AND 1947.” Behavior Genetics, vol. 35, no. 5, 2005.
  8. DOUGLAS, J. W. B. et al. “THE RELATION BETWEEN HEIGHT AND MEASURED EDUCATIONAL ABILITY IN SCHOOL CHILDREN OF THE SAME SOCIAL CLASS, FAMILY SIZE AND STAGE OF SEXUAL DEVELOPMENT.” Human Biology, vol. 37, no. 2, 1965.
  9. 1 2 WILSON, D. M. et al. “GROWTH AND INTELLECTUAL DEVELOPMENT.” Pediatrics, vol. 78, no. 4, 1986.
  10. 1 2 RICCARDO, E. M. et al. “COMMON GENETIC VARIATIONS EXPLAIN THE MAJORITY OF THE CORRELATION BETWEEN HEIGHT AND INTELLIGENCE: THE GENERATION SCOTLAND STUDY.” Behavior Genetics, vol. 44, no. 2, 2014.
  11. Walker SP, Grantham-McGregor SM, Powell CA, Chang SM. Effects of growth restriction in early childhood on growth, IQ, and cognition at age 11 to 12 years and the benefits of nutritional supplementation and psychosocial stimulation. J Pediatr 2000; 137:36–41. Abstract
  12. Wilson, D. M.; Hammer, L. D.; Duncan, P. M.; Dornbusch, S. M.; Ritter, P. L.; Hintz, R. L.; Gross, R. T.; Rosenfeld, R. G. (1986-10-01). "Growth and intellectual development". Pediatrics. 78 (4): 646–650. ISSN 0031-4005. PMID 3763275.
  13. Walker, S. P.; Grantham-Mcgregor, S. M.; Powell, C. A.; Chang, S. M. (2000-07-01). "Effects of growth restriction in early childhood on growth, IQ, and cognition at age 11 to 12 years and the benefits of nutritional supplementation and psychosocial stimulation". The Journal of Pediatrics. 137 (1): 36–41. doi:10.1067/mpd.2000.106227. ISSN 0022-3476. PMID 10891819.
  14. Pearce, Mark S.; Deary, Ian J.; Young, Allan H.; Parker, Louise (2005-06-01). "Growth in early life and childhood IQ at age 11 years: the Newcastle Thousand Families Study". International Journal of Epidemiology. 34 (3): 673–677. doi:10.1093/ije/dyi038. ISSN 0300-5771. PMID 15746206.
  15. Humphreys, L. G.; Davey, T. C.; Park, R. K. (1985-12-01). "Longitudinal correlation analysis of standing height and intelligence". Child Development. 56 (6): 1465–1478. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 4075869.
  16. Tuvemo, T.; Jonsson, B.; Persson, I. (1999-01-01). "Intellectual and physical performance and morbidity in relation to height in a cohort of 18-year-old Swedish conscripts". Hormone Research. 52 (4): 186–191. doi:10.1159/000023459. ISSN 0301-0163. PMID 10725784.
  17. Beauchamp, Jonathan P.; Cesarini, David; Johannesson, Magnus; Lindqvist, Erik; Apicella, Coren (2011-03-01). "On the sources of the height-intelligence correlation: new insights from a bivariate ACE model with assortative mating". Behavior Genetics. 41 (2): 242–252. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9376-7. ISSN 1573-3297. PMC 3044837Freely accessible. PMID 20603722.
  18. Egolf, Donald B.; Corder, Lloyd E. "Height differences of low and high job status, female and male corporate employees". Sex Roles. 24 (5-6): 365–373. doi:10.1007/BF00288309. ISSN 0360-0025.
  19. Sundet, Jon Martin; Tambs, Kristian; Harris, Jennifer R.; Magnus, Per; Torjussen, Tore M. (2005-08-01). "Resolving the Genetic and Environmental Sources of the Correlation Between Height and Intelligence: A Study of Nearly 2600 Norwegian Male Twin Pairs". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 8 (4): 307–311. doi:10.1375/twin.8.4.307. ISSN 1839-2628.
  20. Teasdale, T. W.; Sørensen, T. I.; Owen, D. R. (1989-05-13). "Fall in association of height with intelligence and educational level". BMJ (Clinical research ed.). 298 (6683): 1292–1293. ISSN 0959-8138. PMC 1836522Freely accessible. PMID 2500201.
  21. Silventoinen, K.; Posthuma, D.; van Beijsterveldt, T.; Bartels, M.; Boomsma, D. I. (2006-11-01). "Genetic contributions to the association between height and intelligence: Evidence from Dutch twin data from childhood to middle age". Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 5 (8): 585–595. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00208.x. ISSN 1601-1848. PMID 17081263.
  22. Neisser, Ulric; Boodoo, Gwyneth; J., Thomas; Bouchard, Jr.; Boykin, A. Wade; Brody, Nathan; Ceci, Stephen J.; Halpern, Diane F.; Loehlin, John C. "Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns.". American Psychologist. 51 (2): 77–101. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.51.2.77.
  23. "How much of human height is genetic and how much is due to nutrition?". Scientific American. Retrieved 2016-11-16.
  24. Gale, Catharine (2005-06-01). "Commentary: Height and intelligence". International Journal of Epidemiology. 34 (3): 678–679. doi:10.1093/ije/dyi064. ISSN 0300-5771. PMID 15831563.
  25. Colom, Roberto; Lluis-Font, Josep M; Andrés-Pueyo, Antonio (2005-01-01). "The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: Supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis". Intelligence. 33 (1): 83–91. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010.
  26. Lampl, M.; Mummert, A.; Schoen, M. (2015-10-01). "Auxological perspectives on 'growth' in DOHaD". Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease. 6 (5): 390–398. doi:10.1017/S2040174415001403. ISSN 2040-1744.
  27. 1 2 3 Kanazawa, Satoshi; Reyniers, Diane J. (2009-01-01). "The role of height in the sex difference in intelligence". The American Journal of Psychology. 122 (4): 527–536. ISSN 0002-9556. PMID 20066931. PDF
  28. 1 2 Kanazawa, Satoshi (2005-08-21). "Big and tall parents have more sons: Further generalizations of the Trivers–Willard hypothesis". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 235 (4): 583–590. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.02.010. PDF
  29. McAskie, M.; Clarke, A. M. (1976-05-01). "Parent-offspring resemblances in intelligence: theories and evidence". British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953). 67 (2): 243–273. ISSN 0007-1269. PMID 779915.
  30. 1 2 Cole, Michael. "The Illusion of Culture-free Intelligence Testing". Co-Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. UC San Diego.
  31. 1 2 Rosenthal, Robert; Jacobson, Lenore. "Pygmalion in the classroom". The Urban Review. 3 (1): 16–20. doi:10.1007/BF02322211. ISSN 0042-0972.
  32. Schank, Roger (1994). Beliefs, Reasoning, and Decision Making: Psycho-Logic in Honor of Bob Abelson. Psychology Press. p. 319.
  33. 1 2 Keller, Matthew C.; Garver-Apgar, Christine E.; Wright, Margaret J.; Martin, Nicholas G.; Corley, Robin P.; Stallings, Michael C.; Hewitt, John K.; Zietsch, Brendan P. (2013-04-04). "The Genetic Correlation between Height and IQ: Shared Genes or Assortative Mating?". PLOS Genetics. 9 (4): e1003451. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1003451. ISSN 1553-7404. PMC 3617178Freely accessible. PMID 23593038.
  34. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Beauchamp J. P.; Cesarini D.; Johannesson M.; Lindqvist E.; Apicella C. (2011). "On the sources of the height–intelligence correlation: New insights from a bivariate ACE model with assortative mating". Behavior Genetics. 41 (2): 242–252. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9376-7. PMC 3044837Freely accessible. PMID 20603722.
  35. 1 2 3 4 Sundet JM, Tambs K, Harris JR, Magnus P, Torjussen TM. Resolving the genetic and environmental sources of the correlation between height and intelligence: a study of nearly 2600 Norwegian male twin pairs. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2005 Aug;8(4):307-11
  36. 1 2 3 4 Silventoinen K.; Posthuma D.; van Beijsterveldt T.; Bartels M.; Boomsma D.I. (2006). "Genetic contributions to the association between height and intelligence: evidence from Dutch twin data from childhood to middle age". Genes, Brain and Behavior. 8: 585–595.
  37. Tanner JM. Relation of body size, intelligence test scores and social circumstances. In: Mussen PH, Largen J, Covington M (eds). Trends and Issues in Developmental Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehard and Winston Inc., 1969.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 11/25/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.